Both cute and threatening images drive narrowing of attention in men and women

  1. Álvarez-San Millán, Andrea
  2. Iglesias, Jaime
  3. Gutkin, Anahí
  4. Olivares, Ela I.
  1. 1 Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
    info

    Universidad Autónoma de Madrid

    Madrid, España

    ROR https://ror.org/01cby8j38

Revista:
Psychological Research

ISSN: 0340-0727 1430-2772

Año de publicación: 2021

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.1007/S00426-021-01548-9 GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso abierto editor

Otras publicaciones en: Psychological Research

Objetivos de desarrollo sostenible

Resumen

Appraisal theories of emotion sustain that stimuli containing high biological relevance preferentially capture our attention, regardless of their valence. In this work, we study the priming effect of both cute and threatening pictures on global/local visual processing. Seventy-eight males and 168 females in different menstrual cycle phases performed the global/local Navon letter task immediately after observing cute (infants and baby animals), threatening (animals in aggressive poses) or neutral pictures. Hierarchical stimuli were made up of global and local letters that could be either congruent (e.g. global H, localH) or incongruent (e.g. global H, localS). While we observed no interaction between affective primes and sex/menstrual cycle phase, each of these variables did have an independent effect on the global/local task. Thus, letter identification was significantly slower in the global task (only) after cute vs. neutral primes. Relevantly, the local-minus-global RT index revealed an attentional narrowing after both cute and threatening primes (vs. neutral primes) in incongruent trials. As for sex effects, a facilitation of global vs. local processing was observed in both sexes. However, women registered slower RTs than men, whereas women in the luteal phase showed faster RTs than those in follicular phase in the local task. This suggests that women, mainly those in their luteal menstrual phase, tend to rely on a more analytical style of processing while attending to hierarchical stimuli. Most importantly, stimuli containing high biological significance drive narrowing of the attentional focus in global/local visual processing, especially in conditions of higher attentional demand.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Álvarez-San Millán, A., Iglesias, J., Gutkin, A., & Olivares, E. I. (2021). Forest before trees: Letter stimulus and sex modulate global precedence in visual perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 21, 546483. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.546483 Article Google Scholar
  • Bellaera, L., & von Mühlenen, A. (2019). Looming fearful stimuli broadens attention in a local-global letter task. In N. Srinivasan (Ed.), Progress in brain research: Vol 247. Emotion and cognition (pp. 47–69). Elsevier. Google Scholar
  • Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Sabatinelli, D., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation II: Sex differences in picture processing. Emotion, 1(3), 300–319. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.1.3.300 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Affective picture processing. In N. Birbaumer & A. Öhman (Eds.), The structure of emotion. Psychological, cognitive and clinical aspects (pp. 48–65). Hogrefe & Huber Publishers. Google Scholar
  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment Manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9 Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
  • Brosch, T., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Beyond fear: Rapid spatial orienting toward positive emotional stimuli. Psychological Science, 19(4), 362–370. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02094.x Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). Erlbaum. Google Scholar
  • Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1998). Anxiety and attentional focusing: Trait, state and hemispheric influences. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 745–761. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00117-2 Article Google Scholar
  • Domachowska, I., Heitmann, C., Deutsch, R., Goschke, T., Scherbaum, S., & Bolte, A. (2016). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention: Registered replication report of Gable and Harmon-Jones (2008). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 50–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.09.003 Article Google Scholar
  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological Science, 19(5), 476–482. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02112.x Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The blues broaden, but the nasty narrows: Attentional consequences of negative affects low and high in motivational intensity. Psychological Science, 21(2), 211–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609359622 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2011). Attentional states influence early neural responses associated with motivational processes: Local vs global attentional scope and N1 amplitude to appetitive stimuli. Biological Psychology, 87, 303–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.007 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2012). Reducing attentional capture of emotion by broadening attention: Increased global attention reduces early electrophysiological responses to negative stimuli. Biological Psychology, 90, 150–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.006 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Gable, P. A., Harmon-Jones, E., & Poole, B. (2015). Anger perceptually and conceptually narrows cognitive scope. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 163–174. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039226 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. Wiley. Google Scholar
  • Herrera, A. Y., Wang, J., & Mather, M. (2019). The gist and details of sex differences in cognition and the brain: How parallels in sex differences across domains are shaped by the locus coeruleus and catecholamine systems. Progress in Neurobiology, 176, 120–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2018.05.005 Article Google Scholar
  • Kringelbach, M. L., Lehtonen, A., Squire, S., Harvey, A. G., Craske, M. G., Holliday, I. E., Green, A. L., Aziz, T. Z., Hansen, P. C., Cornelissen, P. L., & Stein, A. (2008). A specific and rapid neural signature for parental instinct. PLoS ONE, 3, e1664. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001664 Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
  • Kringelbach, M. L., Stark, E. A., Alexander, C., Bornstein, M. H., & Stein, A. (2016). On cuteness: Unlocking the parental brain and beyond. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(7), 545–558. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.003 Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical report A-8. University of Florida. Google Scholar
  • Lee, J., Chung, D., Chang, S., Kim, S., Kim, S.-W., Park, H., Ryu, S., & Jeong, J. (2012). Gender differences revealed in the right posterior temporal areas during Navon letter identification tasks. Brain Imaging and Behaviour, 6, 387–396. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-012-9153-8 Article Google Scholar
  • Lorenz, K. (1986). Fundamentos de la etología: Estudio comparado de las conductas (R. Bein, Trans.). Paidós (original work published 1978). Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (2005). Detection Theory (2nd ed.). Lawrence. Google Scholar
  • Meyer, D. E., Irwin, D. E., Osman, A. M., & Kounios, J. (1988). The dynamics of cognition and action: Mental processes inferred from speed accuracy decomposition. Psychological Review, 95, 183–237. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.183 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Moltó, J., Montañés, S., Poy, R., Segarra, P., Pastor, M. C., Tormo, M. P., Ramírez, I., Hernández, M. A., Sánchez, M., Fernández, M. C., & Vila, J. (1999). Un nuevo método para el estudio experimental de las emociones: El “International Affective Picture System” (IAPS). Adaptación española. Revista De Psicología General y Aplicada, 52, 55–87. Google Scholar
  • Moltó, J., Segarra, P., López, R., Esteller, A., Fonfría, M., Pastor, M. C., & Poy, R. (2013). Adaptación Española del “International Affective Picture System” (IAPS). Tercera Parte. Anales De Psicología, 29(3), 965–984. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.29.3.153591 Article Google Scholar
  • Müller-Oehring, E. M., Schulte, T., Raassi, C., Pfefferbaum, A., & Sullivan, E. V. (2007). Local–global interference is modulated by age, sex and anterior corpus callosum size. Brain Research, 1142, 189–205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.062 Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
  • Navon, N. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3 Article Google Scholar
  • Nittono, H. (2016). The two-layer model of ‘kawaii’: A behavioural science framework for understanding kawaii and cuteness. East Asian Journal of Popular Culture, 2(1), 79–95. https://doi.org/10.1386/eapc.2.1.79_1 Article Google Scholar
  • Nittono, H., Fukushima, M., Yano, A., & Moriya, H. (2012). The power of kawaii: Viewing cute images promotes a careful behavior and narrows attentional focus. PLoS ONE, 7(9), e46362. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0046362 Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
  • Noguchi, Y., & Tomoike, K. (2016). Strongly-motivated positive affects induce faster responses to local than global information of visual stimuli: An approach using large-size Navon letters. Scientific Reports, 6, 19136. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep19136 Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
  • Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108(3), 483–522. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.108.3.483 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Olofsson, J. K., Nordin, S., Sequeira, H., & Polich, J. (2008). Affective picture processing: An integrative review of ERP findings. Biological Psychology, 77(3), 247–265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.006 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Parsons, C. E., Young, K. S., Kumari, N., Stein, A., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2011). The motivational salience of infant faces is similar for men and women. PLoS ONE, 6(5), e20632. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020632 Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
  • Pletzer, B., & Harris, T. (2018). Sex hormones modulate the relationship between global advantage, lateralization, and interhemispheric connectivity in a Navon Paradigm. Brain Connectivity, 8(2), 106–118. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2017.0504 Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
  • Pletzer, B., Petasis, O., & Cahill, L. (2014). Switching between forest and trees: Opposite relationship of progesterone and testosterone to global–local processing. Hormones and Behaviour, 66, 257–266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.05.004 Article Google Scholar
  • Razumnikova, O., & Volf, N. (2011). Information processing specialization during interference between global and local aspects of visual hierarchical stimuli in men and women. Fiziologiya Cheloveka, 37(2), 14–19. https://doi.org/10.1134/S0362119711020186 Article Google Scholar
  • Reed, B. G., & Carr, B. R., et al. (2018). The normal menstrual cycle and the control of ovulation. In K. R. Feingold, B. Anawalt, A. Boyce, G. Chrousos, W. W. de Herder, & K. Dungan (Eds.), Endotext. MDText.com, Inc. Google Scholar
  • Roalf, D., Lowery, N., & Turetsky, B. I. (2006). Behavioural and physiological findings of gender differences in global/local visual processing. Brain and Cognition, 60, 32–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2005.09.008 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Russell, J. A., & Pratt, G. (1980). A description of the affective quality attributed to environments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(2), 311–322. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.38.2.311 Article Google Scholar
  • Santos, I. M., Iglesias, J., Olivares, E. I., & Young, A. W. (2008). Differential effects of object-based attention on evoked potentials to fearful and disgusted faces. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1468–1479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.024 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press. Google Scholar
  • Vila, J., Sánchez, M., Ramírez, I., Fernández, M. C., Cobos, P., Rodríguez, S., Muñoz, M. A., Tormo, M. P., Herrero, M., Segarra, P., Pastor, M. C., Montañés, S., Poy, R., & Moltó, J. (2001). El Sistema Internacional de Imágenes Afectivas (IAPS): Adaptación española. Segunda parte. Revista De Psicología General y Aplicada, 54, 635–657. Google Scholar
  • von Mühlenen, A., Bellaera, L., Singh, A., & Srinivasan, N. (2018). The effect of sadness on global/local processing. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 80(5), 1072–1082. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-018-1534-7 Article Google Scholar
  • Wang, H., Chen, Y., & Zhang, Q. (2018). The effects of low and high levels of sadness on scope of attention: And ERP study. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2397. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02397 Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
  • Wu, L., Gu, R., Shi, X., Wang, B., & Zhang, J. (2020). Boosting attachment security to cope with threats: Behavioral and ERPs findings. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 149, 8–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.01.003 Article PubMed Google Scholar
  • Yonkers, K. A., O’Brien, P. S., & Eriksson, E. (2008). Premenstrual syndrome. Lancet, 371(9619), 1200–1210. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60527-9